Aibrary Logo
Podcast thumbnail

The SAT's Original Sin

12 min

Golden Hook & Introduction

SECTION

Sophia: Okay, Laura, I have a question for you. What if I told you that the SAT—the test that has defined college admissions for generations—is only marginally better at predicting your first-year college performance than just looking at your high school grades? Laura: I’d say that sounds about right, and it probably costs a lot more. Sophia: Exactly. But here’s the kicker. What if I told you the man who invented the SAT, Carl Brigham, later denounced his own creation, essentially calling the whole enterprise a psychological "sin"? Laura: Now that is a story. It’s the kind of deep, uncomfortable truth about education that we rarely talk about, but it’s at the very heart of the book we’re diving into today: The RoutledgeFalmer Reader in Psychology of Education, edited by Harry Daniels and Anne Edwards. Sophia: That’s a mouthful. It sounds very academic. Laura: It is, but don't let the title fool you. It's not a dry textbook. It’s a collection of clashing ideas about what education is even for. And the editors, Daniels and Edwards, are both major figures in education who champion what's called a sociocultural or Vygotskian perspective. Sophia: Okay, I'm going to need you to unpack 'Vygotskian' for me later. Laura: Absolutely. But the key thing to know is that they push back hard against the very idea that intelligence is something you can pin down with a number. The book essentially presents two competing dreams for education. The first is the one we all know: education as a giant sorting machine. The second is something far more radical and, I think, far more hopeful.

The Sorting Machine: Education's 'IQ Dream'

SECTION

Sophia: Alright, let's start with the sorting machine. You mentioned the inventor of the SAT regretted it. Unpack that for me. What on earth happened there? Laura: It’s a fascinating and frankly disturbing story, laid out in one of the chapters by Paul Black. The SAT wasn't born in a vacuum. It grew out of the IQ tests developed for the US Army during World War I to quickly sort millions of conscripts. The inventor, Carl Brigham, adapted that military test for universities. Sophia: So it was designed for military efficiency, not necessarily for educational nuance. Laura: Precisely. And the thinking behind it was… ambitious, to put it mildly. The early 20th century was the era of the "IQ dream." Psychologists like Lewis Terman, a key figure in this movement, held some pretty extreme beliefs. The book includes his chilling quote: "all feeble-minded are at least potential criminals." Sophia: Whoa. Hold on. So the foundation of modern intelligence testing had links to eugenics? Laura: It's an uncomfortable part of the history, but yes. The idea was that if you could scientifically measure intelligence, you could engineer a better, more efficient society. The first head of the Educational Testing Service, Henry Chauncey, had this incredible vision. He dreamed of a "Census of all Abilities"—a system to assess every American on every dimension, not just to get them into college, but, and this is a direct quote from the book, "to plot the whole course of their lives." Sophia: That is genuinely terrifying. It's less about education and more about social control. So, did this grand experiment in sorting people actually work? Is the SAT the great predictor of success it claims to be? Laura: Well, that's where the dream collides with reality. The book cites research showing the correlation between SAT scores and first-year college performance is only about 0.5. Sophia: What does that mean in plain English? Laura: It means the SAT accounts for about 25% of the variation in grades. To put that in perspective, your high school grades alone account for about 33%. So the multi-billion dollar testing industry gives you a tiny bit more information, but not much. And it comes with huge costs. Sophia: What kind of costs? Laura: The most obvious one is the rise of test-prep, which proves the test measures coaching as much as aptitude. But the deeper cost is what it does to the classroom. The book shares a powerful story that illustrates this perfectly. It’s about a primary school teacher in the UK, let's call her Mrs. Thompson, who was under immense pressure to raise scores on national literacy tests. Sophia: I can already feel the stress. Laura: Right. The school implemented a strict "teaching to the test" policy. So Mrs. Thompson had to ditch creative activities, critical thinking, and social-emotional learning to just drill grammar rules and test-format exercises. She had a student, Alex, who was bright but struggled with reading. He started falling further and further behind because she just didn't have the time for the individualized attention he needed. Sophia: Oh, that’s heartbreaking. What happened to him? Laura: His parents, seeing his frustration, eventually got him assessed by an outside psychologist who diagnosed him with dyslexia. With the right support, he thrived. But the story shows the damage of the sorting machine model. In the relentless pursuit of a measurable score, the actual child—his needs, his development, his unique mind—got completely lost. The system wasn't designed to help Alex learn; it was designed to measure him, and in doing so, it almost failed him.

The Social Brain: Intelligence as a Team Sport

SECTION

Sophia: Okay, so if the 'sorting machine' model is so flawed and can cause real harm, what's the alternative that Daniels and Edwards and the other authors in this reader are proposing? Laura: They propose a fundamentally different dream. It’s built on the work of a Russian psychologist named Lev Vygotsky, and the core idea is simple but profound: intelligence isn't a fixed thing inside your skull. It's a social practice. It's something we build together. Sophia: Intelligence as a team sport? I like that. But what does it actually mean? Laura: Vygotsky’s most famous concept is the "Zone of Proximal Development," or ZPD. It’s the sweet spot of learning. It’s not what you can do on your own, and it’s not what’s completely beyond your reach. The ZPD is the range of things you can do with help—with guidance from a teacher, a parent, or even a peer. Learning happens when someone helps you cross that zone. Sophia: So it’s about collaboration, not just solo grinding. Laura: Exactly. And this changes how we should think about assessment. If you only test what a child can do alone, you’re only measuring their past learning. You’re not measuring their potential to grow. The book highlights a brilliant experiment that shows this perfectly. It’s a takedown of one of the most famous experiments by the other giant of developmental psychology, Jean Piaget. Sophia: Piaget! I remember him. The stages of development, right? Laura: The very same. Piaget had this famous "conservation task." You show a child two identical glasses with the same amount of water. Then, you pour the water from one glass into a tall, skinny glass. You ask the child, "Which one has more?" Young kids almost always point to the tall glass. Piaget concluded they lack the logical ability to "conserve" quantity. Sophia: Right, they get fooled by the height. Laura: But then, as the book details, some researchers in the 70s, McGarrigle and Donaldson, questioned this. They wondered if the kids were just trying to please the adult. The act of pouring and asking the question implies something has changed. So they redesigned the experiment. They introduced a "naughty teddy bear." Sophia: A naughty teddy bear? I love this already. Laura: The teddy bear would "accidentally" knock over the glass and spill the water into the tall, skinny one. The experimenter would say, "Oh, that silly teddy! He's messed up the game!" Then they'd ask the child the same question. And guess what happened? Sophia: The kids got it right? Laura: The vast majority of them did! Because the social context changed. It wasn't a test anymore; it was a game. The transformation was accidental, not deliberate. By changing the social story around the task, they revealed a competence that Piaget's sterile, individual test had completely hidden. Sophia: Wow. That's incredible. It’s not that the kids were illogical; it’s that the original test was socially awkward! It’s like when you’re stuck on a problem, and just talking it through with a friend helps you see the answer, even if they don't give it to you. The conversation itself unlocks something. Laura: That is the perfect analogy! And it’s the foundation for cooperative learning, which the book explores in depth. It’s the idea that one of the most powerful ways to learn something is to have to explain it to someone else. Sophia: What does that look like in a real classroom? Is it just those group projects where one kid inevitably does all the work while the others coast? Laura: That's the risk, and the book is honest about it. It discusses methods designed to avoid that. One famous one is called the "Jigsaw" method. The teacher breaks a topic into, say, four parts. The class is divided into groups of four, and each person in the group is responsible for learning one part. Sophia: So you become the designated expert on your piece. Laura: Exactly. Then, all the "experts" on Part 1 get together to master their section. Then they return to their original Jigsaw groups and teach their piece to their teammates. To succeed on the final quiz, you need to have learned from everyone in your group. Sophia: That’s clever. It forces real interdependence. Laura: It does. But the book also adds a crucial, nuanced critique. Research shows the achievement outcomes for Jigsaw can be mixed. Why? Because while you become a great expert on your own piece, you might only get a superficial understanding of your teammates' pieces. It shows that even this more humanistic, social model isn't a magic bullet. It requires thoughtful design and skilled facilitation. It's messier than just handing out a standardized test.

Synthesis & Takeaways

SECTION

Sophia: So this leaves us in a really interesting place. We have these two completely different models of education clashing. On one side, there's the 'IQ dream'—the sorting machine. It's simple, it's scalable, it gives you a number, but it seems deeply flawed and potentially harmful. Laura: It narrows the definition of intelligence to what is easily measured. Sophia: And on the other side, we have this social, collaborative model. It feels more human, more effective, and more aligned with how we actually learn in the real world. But as the Jigsaw example shows, it's complex and much harder to implement. What's the big takeaway from this reader? Laura: I think the ultimate insight from Daniels and Edwards' collection is that the 'sorting machine' dream, the one based on standardized testing, offers a false promise of simplicity. It’s seductive to think we can capture a person's potential in a single score. But it's a reductionist view that can distort the very purpose of education. Sophia: It mistakes the map for the territory. Laura: A perfect way to put it. The more hopeful, and I think more accurate, vision is that intelligence isn't a static object to be measured, like your height. It's a dynamic capacity that is cultivated in community. It’s Vygotskian to its core. The interactions we have, the tools we use, the culture we're in—all of it shapes our cognitive abilities. Sophia: So the goal of education shouldn't be to find the 'smart' kids. Laura: The goal should be to create environments where everyone can become smarter, together. The real work of education isn't just assessing individuals in isolation; it's designing the rich, social, sometimes messy, collaborative experiences that allow intelligence to emerge and flourish. It’s about building a better garden, not a more efficient factory. Sophia: That’s a powerful reframe. It shifts the focus from measurement to creation. Laura: It does. And it leaves us with a really potent question. So the next time you see a test score, or hear someone labeled as 'smart' or 'not smart,' it's worth asking: what is this number or this label really measuring? And more importantly, what is it missing about the vast, social, and collaborative nature of human potential? Sophia: What would our schools, our workplaces, and even our families look like if we stopped trying to measure people and started focusing on creating the conditions for them to learn from each other? Laura: That's the question. Sophia: This is Aibrary, signing off.

00:00/00:00