
The Psychology of Intelligence
9 minIntroduction
Narrator: A six-year-old child is asked who is older, them or their father. The child correctly states the father is older. But when asked if the father was born before or after them, the child insists the father was born after they were. To the child, a time before their own existence is simply inconceivable. This baffling logic is not a sign of a defect, but a window into a different world of thought, a world where reality is constructed according to a unique set of rules.
This is the world meticulously mapped by the pioneering psychologist Jean Piaget. In his foundational work, The Psychology of Intelligence, Piaget dismantles the idea of intelligence as a fixed quantity we possess. Instead, he reveals it as a living, evolving process—a biological function that allows us to adapt and make sense of our environment. The book provides a revolutionary journey into the mind of a child, showing how the sophisticated logic of an adult is built, step-by-step, from the most basic sensory experiences.
Intelligence Is a Process of Adaptation, Not a Fixed Trait
Key Insight 1
Narrator: Before Piaget, intelligence was often seen as a static attribute, something you either had a lot of or a little of. Piaget reframed this entirely. He argued that intelligence is fundamentally a biological process of adaptation. It’s the primary tool organisms use to achieve a state of equilibrium with their environment. This adaptation isn't passive; it's a dynamic dance between two complementary actions: assimilation and accommodation.
Assimilation is the process of taking in new information and fitting it into pre-existing mental structures, or "schemata." Imagine a toddler who has a schema for "dog" that includes having four legs, fur, and a tail. When they see a cat for the first time, they might point and say "dog!" They are assimilating this new creature into their existing framework.
Accommodation, however, is the other side of the coin. It's the process of altering those existing schemata to make sense of new information that doesn't quite fit. When the parent corrects the toddler, explaining that this animal is a "cat" because it meows instead of barks, the child must accommodate. They adjust their understanding, creating a new schema for "cat" or refining their "dog" schema to be more specific. For Piaget, intelligence is this constant, active process of balancing assimilation and accommodation to build an increasingly complex and accurate model of the world.
The Mind Is Built Through Action
Key Insight 2
Narrator: Piaget demonstrated that the origins of thought are not abstract but are rooted in physical action. The first stage of cognitive development, lasting from birth to about two years, is called the sensorimotor stage. During this period, an infant learns about the world exclusively through its senses and motor activities—touching, tasting, seeing, and manipulating.
A cornerstone achievement of this stage is the development of "object permanence," the understanding that things continue to exist even when they are out of sight. Piaget famously documented this process by observing his own son, Laurent. When Laurent was very young, if Piaget hid a toy under a blanket, the infant would act as if the toy had vanished from existence. He made no effort to search for it. The world was a series of fleeting perceptual displays.
As Laurent grew, he began to search for partially hidden objects. By around eight months, he would search for a toy that was completely hidden. However, his understanding was still fragile. Piaget would hide the toy under a cloth at location A, and Laurent would find it. After repeating this several times, Piaget would then, in full view of Laurent, hide the toy under a new cloth at location B. Astonishingly, Laurent would still search for it at location A. This "A-not-B error" shows that the infant's concept of the object is still tied to their own action of finding it, not to its independent existence in space. Only through continued trial and error—through action—does the child eventually build a stable, independent concept of the object.
From Reflex to Reason
Key Insight 3
Narrator: The journey from a newborn's reflexive world to a child's conceptual one is a process of construction. Piaget argues that at the most basic level, the reflex level, there are no "objects" at all. An infant seeking the breast is not responding to a permanent, substantial object. They are responding to a situation, a collection of sensations and stimuli that trigger a reflexive action. The breast, as an independent entity, does not yet exist in the infant's mind.
The concept of an object is an intellectual and perceptual achievement. As Piaget states, "To have the concept of an object is to attribute the perceived figure to a substantial basis, so that the figure and the substance that it thus indicates continue to exist outside the perceptual field." This is the first great act of conservation—the understanding that something remains the same despite changes in perception.
This construction is not purely mental; it is deeply intertwined with motor habits. The infant's ability to follow an object with their eyes, to reach for it, and to manipulate it are all actions that help build the schema of a permanent object. Intelligence, therefore, grows directly out of these sensorimotor functions, transforming basic physical habits into the building blocks of abstract thought.
Logic Is a Social Achievement
Key Insight 4
Narrator: While the early stages of intelligence are built through individual action, Piaget makes it clear that higher-level logical thought cannot develop in a vacuum. It is fundamentally a social process. A young child's thought is characterized by egocentrism—an inability to differentiate their own viewpoint from that of others.
This is vividly illustrated in a simple experiment. A five-year-old can easily identify their own right and left hands. But if an adult sits opposite them and asks them to point to the adult's right hand, the child will often point to the hand that is on the same side as their own right hand. They cannot yet perform the mental rotation required to adopt another person's spatial perspective. Their reality is the only reality.
Language itself is assimilated egocentrically. A child whose only experience with a "bird" is a pet canary will initially use the word "bird" to mean only canaries. They must learn through social exchange and exposure that the concept is broader.
It is through social interaction—through argument, cooperation, and the need to explain oneself to others—that the child is forced to "decenter." They are confronted with contradictions and alternative viewpoints. This pressure requires them to build a system of thought that is not just internally consistent but also consistent with the views of others. Logic, in this sense, is not just a set of abstract rules but, as Piaget puts it, a "morality of thinking." It’s a shared system of norms that makes intellectual exchange and cooperation possible. The obligation to avoid contradicting oneself is a social imperative before it is a logical one. Operational groupings and cooperation are two sides of the same coin; one cannot exist without the other.
Conclusion
Narrator: The single most important takeaway from Jean Piaget's The Psychology of Intelligence is that intelligence is not something we are born with, but something we construct. It is a developmental marvel, a journey that takes us from the reflexive actions of an infant, to the action-based logic of a toddler, and finally to the abstract, social reasoning of an adult. The "errors" in a child's thinking are not failures; they are the necessary and brilliant steps in the process of building a mind.
Piaget's work challenges us to look at children not as empty vessels to be filled with facts, but as active scientists, constantly experimenting, forming hypotheses, and building their understanding of reality from the ground up. The next time you see a child making a seemingly illogical mistake, perhaps you will see it differently: not as an error to be corrected, but as a beautiful, fleeting glimpse into the workshop of the human mind.