
Your Brain on Money
12 minThe Psychology of Money and How to Use It Better
Golden Hook & Introduction
SECTION
Michelle: A study of sugar cane farmers in India found their IQ scores dropped by ten points when they were worried about money. It wasn't that they became less intelligent; their financial stress was literally eating up their brainpower. What if poverty isn't a lack of character, but a cognitive burden? Mark: Wait, ten points? That's a huge drop. That’s the difference between 'average' and 'borderline gifted' on some scales. You’re saying just the worry about money can do that? Michelle: Exactly. It’s this incredible concept of "cognitive bandwidth." And it’s the perfect entry point into the book we’re exploring today: Mind Over Money: The Psychology of Money and How to Use It Better by Claudia Hammond. Mark: I like that title. It’s not about spreadsheets and stocks, it’s about the mind. Michelle: And the author, Claudia Hammond, is perfect for this. She's not an economist; she's a psychology lecturer and a long-time BBC presenter for a show called All in the Mind. Her whole career is about making complex psychology accessible, which is exactly what this book does. It’s packed with over 250 studies, but it reads like a collection of fascinating stories about why we are so weird about money. Mark: Okay, so where do you even start with something so huge? Is it about how we spend, or something deeper? Michelle: It's much deeper. It starts with the very idea of money itself. Which brings me to one of the wildest stories I've ever read, and it’s the book’s opening hook. In 1994, the British band KLF did something that caused a national outrage. Mark: More outrageous than their music? Michelle: Much more. They took one million pounds in cash—their entire earnings—to an abandoned boathouse on a remote Scottish island, and they filmed themselves burning it. All of it.
The Abstract Spell: Why Money Isn't Real, But Controls Everything
SECTION
Mark: Oh, I feel sick just hearing that. I have a physical reaction to that. Why does that feel so much worse than them just, say, buying a million-dollar yacht and immediately crashing it? It’s the same amount of waste, isn't it? Michelle: That’s the exact question Hammond wants us to ask! And the answer is no, it’s not the same. When you crash a yacht, you destroy a thing. When you burn a million pounds, you’re destroying pure potential. Mark: Potential. What do you mean? Michelle: That pile of paper could have become anything. It could have been a donation to a hospital, an investment in a new business, a thousand family holidays, or even just a ridiculously lavish party. By burning it, the KLF destroyed every single one of those possibilities. They violated a fundamental social contract. Hammond argues money isn't just paper or numbers; it's a system of mutual trust. It’s a mental construct that we all agree has value. Mark: A shared delusion, almost. Michelle: A very effective one! The book mentions the historical example of the Yap islanders in the Pacific, who used giant, immovable stone discs as currency. One time, a huge stone disc was being transported by canoe and fell to the bottom of the ocean. Everyone on the island agreed it was still there and still belonged to its owner's family, so it continued to be used in transactions for generations. Its value was purely in their collective belief. Mark: Wow. So the KLF didn't just burn paper, they burned trust. They took this symbol of infinite possibility and reduced it to literal ash. Michelle: Precisely. And that visceral reaction you had is proof of how deeply we’ve internalized that belief. But here’s the modern twist. You said you barely use cash anymore. Does this still apply to our credit cards and digital wallets? Mark: That’s a great question. I definitely don't feel the same "pain" when I tap my card as when I hand over a fifty-dollar bill. It feels less real. Michelle: Hammond covers this perfectly. There was a classic experiment at MIT where students were bidding on basketball tickets. One group was told they had to pay in cash, the other with a credit card. The results were staggering. Mark: Let me guess, the credit card group bid way more. Michelle: Not just more. Double. The cash payers bid an average of around $28. The card payers were willing to go up to $60. The physical act of handing over cash—the "pain of paying"—is a powerful psychological brake. With cards, that friction is gone. Money becomes even more of an abstraction, which makes us spend it more freely, and often, more foolishly. Mark: And that’s how you end up with a subscription to a cheese-of-the-month club you don't remember signing up for. The lack of friction is dangerous. Michelle: It is. And that friction, or lack thereof, is just one of dozens of mental glitches that dictate our financial lives, which is the second huge idea in this book.
The Biased Brain: How Mental Glitches Skew Our Financial Choices
SECTION
Mark: Okay, so our brains are not wired for modern finance. What are some of these other glitches? Michelle: The biggest one is something called "mental accounting." Hammond gives a perfect, simple example. Imagine you’re on holiday and you want to hire a bike for the day. The shop quotes you £25. But then you hear there's another shop ten minutes' walk away that has the same bike for £10. Do you make the walk to save £15? Mark: Absolutely. That’s a huge saving, more than half off. It’s a no-brainer. Michelle: Okay. Now, imagine you’re buying a new car. The showroom quotes you £10,025. But you find out another showroom, also ten minutes away, will sell you the exact same car for £10,010. Do you make that ten-minute trip to save £15? Mark: Huh. Honestly? Probably not. It feels like a rounding error on a purchase that big. It’s not worth the hassle. But… wait a minute. It’s the same £15. And the same ten-minute walk. Michelle: Exactly! Welcome to mental accounting. Your brain doesn't treat all money as equal. It puts money into different mental "jars" or "accounts." The £15 saving on the bike feels huge because it’s coming from your small "daily entertainment" jar. The £15 saving on the car feels insignificant because it’s part of the enormous "major life purchase" jar. Mark: Wow. So I’m not making a rational calculation about the value of my time versus the money saved. I’m making an emotional judgment based on the context. I’ve 100% done that. I'll drive across town to save $5 on a t-shirt but won't even blink at a $200 "delivery fee" on a new sofa. Michelle: We all do. It's a cognitive bias. And it’s paired with another powerful one: loss aversion. The research, pioneered by psychologists Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky, shows that the pain of losing is psychologically about twice as powerful as the pleasure of gaining. Mark: So we hate losing $20 more than we enjoy finding $20. Michelle: Far more. This leads to all sorts of strange behavior. The book describes experiments with capuchin monkeys, who share a common ancestor with us from 35 million years ago. They were taught to use tokens to "buy" grapes. One researcher would consistently give them one grape. Another would show them two grapes, but then take one away before giving them the remaining one. Mark: So the monkeys got one grape either way. Michelle: Correct. But the monkeys quickly learned to despise the researcher who showed them two and then took one away. They were experiencing loss aversion. They would actively avoid the "loss" trader, even though the outcome was identical. That instinct is ancient. Mark: That is incredible. It’s not learned behavior; it’s baked into our primate DNA. So are these biases just harmless quirks, or do they have serious real-world consequences? Michelle: They have huge consequences. Think about the anchoring effect. The first number you hear in a negotiation heavily influences the outcome. Hammond points to a really insidious example: the minimum payment on a credit card bill. Mark: Right, the tiny number they suggest you pay. Michelle: That number acts as a powerful anchor. It’s intended to be a safety net, but psychologically, it makes people pay off far less than they otherwise would. It reframes their perception of what a "reasonable" payment is. That one little number, that one mental glitch, can be the difference between paying off debt quickly and being trapped for years. Mark: It feels like all these biases are setting us up to fail. Does having more money just make us better at navigating them, or does it actually... change us as people? Michelle: That is the million-dollar question, isn't it? And it leads us right to the book's most profound and sometimes controversial territory: the relationship between money, motivation, and our moral compass.
The Moral Compass: Does Money Make Us Good, Bad, or Just... Different?
SECTION
Mark: Okay, so the classic assumption is that money is the ultimate motivator. You want someone to do something? Pay them. More money, more motivation. Simple. Michelle: You would think so. But Hammond presents some fascinating research that completely flips that idea on its head. There was a study in Israel with high school students who go door-to-door collecting for charity. It's a big tradition, a point of pride. Mark: A classic act of altruism. Michelle: Exactly. So researchers decided to test the power of incentives. They divided the students into three groups. Group one was the control group; they were just given a motivational speech about the charity's importance. Group two was offered a small commission, 1% of what they collected. Group three was offered a much larger 10% commission. Which group do you think raised the most money? Mark: Well, based on everything we think we know, the 10% group. Obviously. Michelle: The group that raised the most money was the one that was paid nothing at all. Mark: What? How is that even possible? They collected less money when they were paid? Michelle: Significantly less. The 1% group performed the worst of all. The researchers call this the "crowding out" effect. Before, the students were operating in the social world, motivated by civic duty, altruism, and peer recognition. The moment you introduce money, you shift them into a market world. And suddenly, their noble act of volunteering becomes a really, really badly paid job. Their intrinsic motivation was "crowded out" by a weak financial one. Mark: So the lesson is: pay enough, or don't pay at all. A tiny payment is actually an insult. It devalues the act itself. Michelle: Precisely. It suggests that our motivation isn't a single dial that money just turns up. We have different, competing motivational systems. And this connects to another counter-intuitive finding: the secret to happiness isn't earning more money, but giving it away. Mark: I’ve heard this before, but does the research actually back it up? Michelle: It does. A study in Vancouver gave people an envelope with either $5 or $20. Half were told to spend it on themselves by the end of the day, and the other half were told to spend it on someone else—a gift, a donation, anything. That evening, the people who spent the money on others reported significantly higher levels of happiness. The amount didn't matter. The act of giving was the key. Mark: That’s a powerful idea. But some readers have pointed out that while the book is full of these fascinating studies, it can feel a bit light on practical, actionable advice. It's a great diagnosis of our money problems, but what's the prescription? Michelle: That's a fair critique, and it’s a common reaction to the book. It can feel like a whirlwind tour of 263 different experiments. But I think Hammond's goal isn't to give you a 5-step budget plan. The big takeaway is that 'financial literacy'—knowing how to make a budget—isn't enough. We need psychological literacy.
Synthesis & Takeaways
SECTION
Mark: Psychological literacy. I like that. So it’s not about being better at math, it's about being more self-aware. Michelle: Exactly. The prescription isn't a set of rules; it's a new lens. It's the ability to step back in a moment of decision and ask: Is this the anchoring effect talking? Am I in a "scarcity tunnel" right now that's making me focus only on the short term? Am I treating this "fun money" differently from my "serious savings" because of mental accounting? Mark: So the real 'Mind Over Money' is knowing your own mind. Recognizing the game your brain is playing is the first and most powerful step to taking back control. Michelle: That’s the core of it. You can’t fight an enemy you can’t see. This book turns on the lights and shows you the hidden wiring in your own head. It gives you the power to notice the biases, and in that moment of noticing, you get a choice. You’re no longer on autopilot. Mark: And that choice is everything. It’s the space between the impulse to buy and the actual purchase. It’s deciding to walk ten minutes to save that £15, not because of the car's price, but because you know your time is worth it. Michelle: Exactly. And we'd love to know which of these money biases you see in your own life. Is it mental accounting? The endowment effect, where you overvalue stuff just because you own it? Let us know your story. We're always curious to hear how these ideas land in the real world. Mark: A fascinating and slightly unsettling look in the mirror. Thanks, Michelle. Michelle: This is Aibrary, signing off.