Aibrary Logo
Podcast thumbnail

Who's Driving Your Brain?

11 min

The Story of You

Golden Hook & Introduction

SECTION

Mark: Most of us think we know ourselves. We feel we're in the driver's seat of our own lives. But what if everything you believe, every choice you make, and even your core personality is being run by a secret, hidden operator in your own head? Michelle: That's a fantastic and deeply unsettling question, Mark. And it's exactly the territory we're exploring today. We're diving into a book that pulls back the curtain on that hidden operator. It's called Incognito: The Secret Lives of the Brain by David Eagleman. Mark: I’ve heard of this one. It was a huge bestseller, right? Michelle: Absolutely. And it's written by David Eagleman, who's this fascinating figure—a top-tier neuroscientist at Stanford, but his undergrad degree was actually in literature. You can feel that storyteller's touch all over this book, which is probably why it became such a massive hit and even a PBS series. He takes these incredibly complex ideas and makes them feel like a gripping story about you. Mark: A story about me that I don't even know is being written. That's a little creepy. Where do we even start with that? Michelle: We start with the most fundamental question of all: Who are you? Eagleman argues that the answer is far stranger and more physical than we imagine. His first big challenge is to our very idea of a stable "self."

The Brain as the Unseen Architect of 'You'

SECTION

Michelle: He has this quote that just stops you in your tracks: "Neurally speaking, who you are depends on where you’ve been." He’s not being metaphorical. He means your identity is physically sculpted by your experiences. Mark: Okay, I can see how experiences shape your perspective, but physically sculpted? What does that actually look like? Michelle: Well, he gives a truly heartbreaking example to show what happens when the sculpting goes wrong. He talks about the Romanian orphanages under the Ceauşescu regime. In the late 20th century, hundreds of thousands of children were warehoused in these state-run institutions. Mark: Oh man, I remember hearing about this. The conditions were horrific. Michelle: Beyond horrific. The children were fed and clothed, but they were deprived of the most crucial ingredient for brain development: human interaction. They weren't held, they weren't spoken to, they weren't played with. They lived in a state of extreme sensory and emotional neglect. When researchers like Dr. Charles Nelson studied these children years later, the findings were devastating. Mark: What did they find? Michelle: They found measurable, physical differences. The children had lower IQs, but more than that, their brain scans showed dramatically reduced neural activity. The parts of their brains responsible for complex thought, emotion, and attachment were quite literally underdeveloped. They were stunted because they lacked the necessary input from the world. Their brains were waiting for experiences that never came. Mark: That's just devastating. So the brain is like a house that needs raw materials from the outside world to even get built? Michelle: Exactly. And without those materials, the structure is weak. But let's flip that. Eagleman provides a powerful counter-example: the London cab drivers. To become a licensed black cab driver in London, you have to pass an exam called "The Knowledge." Mark: I’ve heard this is insanely difficult. Michelle: It's one of the hardest memory tests in the world. They have to memorize 25,000 streets and thousands of landmarks within a six-mile radius of Charing Cross. It takes years of driving around on a moped, just memorizing. So neuroscientists at University College London had a brilliant idea: scan their brains. Mark: And what did they find? I'm guessing it wasn't a tiny GPS chip. Michelle: Nothing like that. They found that the posterior hippocampus—a part of the brain that's vital for spatial memory—was significantly larger in the cab drivers compared to a control group. And here’s the kicker: the longer they had been on the job, the bigger that part of their brain was. Mark: Whoa. So in one case, a lack of experience stunts the brain, and in the other, intense experience physically grows it. The brain is literally being sculpted, like clay. Michelle: Precisely. And this sculpting process is most dramatic in childhood. Eagleman uses this beautiful quote: "You become who you are not because of what grows in your brain, but because of what is removed." Mark: What does that mean? Removed? Michelle: It’s a process called neural pruning. When you're a baby, your brain overproduces connections, creating a dense, tangled jungle of possibilities. Then, based on your experiences, the connections you use get stronger, and the ones you don't use get "pruned" away. Your brain customizes itself to your specific world. Mark: So I'm less a work of art and more a result of aggressive editing? That's... less romantic. Michelle: A bit, but it’s incredibly efficient! The brain wires itself to be an expert in your environment, whether that's recognizing your parents' faces, learning your native language, or navigating the streets of London. Who you are is the result of that custom-built hardware.

The Illusion of Conscious Control

SECTION

Mark: Okay, so our brain is this physical thing, this piece of hardware, that's shaped by the world. That I can sort of accept. But I'm still the one making my choices, right? I decide to pick up this coffee cup. I am the software running on the hardware. Michelle: Are you, though? Eagleman would say that even that simple act of lifting your coffee cup is a performance of staggering complexity, almost all of which happens unconsciously. Your visual system locates the cup, your memory recalls its weight and temperature, your frontal cortex signals your motor cortex, which orchestrates a symphony of muscle contractions. Your nerves are sending a constant stream of feedback about its position and slipperiness, and your brain is making micro-adjustments in real-time. Mark: And I'm aware of exactly none of that. I just think, "I want coffee." Michelle: Exactly. The conscious you is like the CEO of a massive corporation who just reads the executive summary. You have no idea about the thousands of employees in the mailroom, in accounting, in R&D, who are actually doing all the work. You just get the final report. Mark: That's a useful analogy, but it's still just about automatic motor skills. What about big, conscious decisions? What about my morality? My actions? Michelle: This is where the book gets really provocative. Eagleman brings up the case of Charles Whitman. On August 1st, 1966, Whitman, a former Eagle Scout and engineering student, climbed a tower at the University of Texas and opened fire, killing 13 people and wounding dozens more. It was a horrific, inexplicable act. Mark: I can't even imagine. What could possibly lead someone to do that? Michelle: Well, in the suicide note he wrote the night before, Whitman complained of overwhelming, irrational, violent thoughts. He couldn't understand them. He even wrote, "I would like an autopsy on me to see if there is any visible physical disorder." Mark: He asked for his own autopsy? That’s chilling. Michelle: And they did it. The pathologist found a small brain tumor, the size of a nickel, pressing against a part of his brain called the amygdala. The amygdala is a key region for regulating fear and aggression. Mark: Hold on. A brain tumor could have made him a mass murderer? That completely changes the conversation about blame and responsibility. Michelle: It throws it into chaos, doesn't it? It raises the question: was it "him" who committed those acts, or was it his biology? If your brain is compromised, are your actions still your own? Eagleman uses this to show that the line between our choices and our physical brain is blurry, or maybe even non-existent. Mark: This is getting into some deep water. It feels like it's chipping away at the idea of free will. Michelle: It gets even more direct. He describes experiments using something called Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, or TMS. It's a device that can temporarily ramp up or tamp down activity in specific brain regions from outside the skull. In one experiment, they ask a person to decide whether to move their left or right hand. Mark: Okay, a simple choice. Michelle: But just before the person "chooses," the scientists use TMS to stimulate the motor area for, say, the left hand. And guess what? The person becomes much more likely to "choose" to move their left hand. But here is the truly mind-bending part: when you ask them why they moved their left hand, they don't say "a magnet made me do it." They say, "I just felt like it," or "I wanted to switch things up." They invent a story to explain a choice that was made for them. Mark: Wow. So not only are our actions driven by unconscious machinery, but our feeling of making a choice is also just another part of the show the brain puts on for us. That's a huge claim. Is this where some critics feel Eagleman might be overstating the case? Michelle: It is. The book is widely acclaimed for its accessibility, but some in the neuroscience and philosophy communities argue that it simplifies the debate on free will. They'd say that showing an influence on a decision isn't the same as disproving free will entirely. But Eagleman's goal, I think, is less about providing a final answer and more about forcing us to confront the evidence. He wants us to see that the "conscious you" is not the sole author of your life story.

Synthesis & Takeaways

SECTION

Mark: So if our identity is sculpted by outside forces and our choices are driven by a hidden unconscious, it feels a bit like we're just puppets. It’s a pretty bleak picture. Michelle: I can see why it feels that way, but Eagleman offers a more empowering perspective. He uses the analogy of the brain as a "neural parliament." There isn't one "you." There are multiple, competing drives and networks in your brain, all vying for control. The rational system wants you to save for retirement; the impulsive system wants that slice of cake right now. The decision you make is simply the outcome of which party won the vote at that moment. Mark: A parliament of rivals in my head. That actually sounds about right, especially on a Tuesday afternoon. Michelle: Exactly! And the takeaway isn't that we're helpless puppets. The point is that if you understand the machinery, you can start to work with it. You can't consciously will yourself to have more willpower, for example. But you can recognize that your future self might be weaker, and you can set things up to help that person out. Mark: Like a Ulysses contract. Tying yourself to the mast so you don't steer your ship into the rocks when the Sirens start singing. Michelle: Precisely. You remove the cookies from the house. You set your workout clothes out the night before. You use software to block distracting websites. You are no longer just the pilot; you become the ship's architect. You're shaping the environment that will, in turn, shape your unconscious decisions. That's a new, more sophisticated kind of control. Mark: That’s a much more hopeful way to look at it. It’s not about fighting your brain, but about understanding the rules of its game and then setting up the playing field in your favor. Michelle: That’s the core of it. The book is a journey into realizing how little we know about ourselves, but it ends with a sense of profound possibility. Mark: It leaves me with a big question, though. If we're not who we think we are, and we're not in control like we think we are... then what does that mean for how we should live our lives? Michelle: That's the billion-dollar question, isn't it? And maybe the first step is just a little more humility. Humility about our own certainty, our own beliefs, and our judgments of others. We're all just trying to navigate the world with the strange computational material in our skulls. We'd love to hear what you all think. Does this idea free you, or does it frighten you? Let us know your thoughts. Mark: This is Aibrary, signing off.

00:00/00:00