Aibrary Logo
Podcast thumbnail

Felt Time

11 min

The Psychology of How We Perceive Time

Introduction

Narrator: Why does a thrilling one-week vacation feel like it's over in a flash, yet in retrospect, it seems packed with more memories and substance than a monotonous month at the office? Why does time seem to drag interminably when we are bored or anxious, but accelerate uncontrollably as we get older? We all feel these distortions, this strange elasticity of time, but we often dismiss them as mere feelings. What if they are not? What if our subjective sense of time is the most fundamental experience we have, a direct perception of our own consciousness unfolding? In his book Felt Time: The Psychology of How We Perceive Time, neuroscientist and psychologist Marc Wittmann argues that our experience of time isn't just a perception of the outside world, but an internal sense generated by the body itself. He reveals that our relationship with time is deeply intertwined with memory, emotion, self-awareness, and the very rhythm of our biology.

The Marshmallow and the Future Self

Key Insight 1

Narrator: One of the most famous experiments in psychology provides a powerful window into our relationship with time. In the late 1960s, psychologist Walter Mischel placed children, one by one, in a room with a single marshmallow. He gave them a choice: they could eat the treat immediately, or, if they could wait for him to return in about fifteen minutes, they would be rewarded with a second marshmallow. This simple test of delayed gratification proved to be remarkably predictive. Years later, the children who had managed to wait for the second marshmallow tended to have better academic results, stronger social skills, and a greater ability to cope with stress.

Wittmann uses this story to illustrate a core human conflict: the battle between our present self and our future self. The inability to wait for the larger reward is a phenomenon known as temporal myopia, or shortsightedness. We tend to devalue future rewards, a concept called temporal discounting. For example, most people would prefer $45 today over $50 in a week, even though the rational choice is to wait. This isn't just about money; it's about our fundamental orientation toward time. Impulsive individuals, who live for immediate gratification, often experience profound boredom because they are constantly seeking the next stimulus. In contrast, those who can delay gratification are investing in their future well-being. This ability to connect with a future version of ourselves, to sacrifice a small pleasure now for a greater one later, is a cornerstone of a successful and fulfilled life.

The Accelerating Clock of Adulthood

Key Insight 2

Narrator: A near-universal human experience is the feeling that time speeds up as we get older. A childhood summer felt like an eternity, but now entire years seem to vanish in the blink of an eye. Wittmann explains that this isn't just a feeling; it's a direct consequence of how our brain encodes memory. Our retrospective judgment of a period's length is based on the number of new, distinct memories we formed during that time. When we are young, nearly everything is a novel experience—the first day of school, a family vacation to a new place, learning to ride a bike. Each of these events creates a rich, dense memory, making time feel expansive in retrospect.

As we age, our lives often fall into a predictable routine. We drive the same route to work, eat similar meals, and interact with the same people. Because the days are so similar, the brain doesn't need to create distinct memories for each one. They blur together into a single, undifferentiated block. This lack of new memory markers is what creates the illusion of acceleration. Thomas Mann captured this perfectly in his novel The Magic Mountain, where the protagonist Hans Castorp's first few days in a sanatorium are described in immense detail, feeling long and full. But as the weeks and months pass and routine sets in, vast stretches of time are covered in just a few sentences. The lesson is clear: a life filled with novelty and varied experiences will feel subjectively longer and richer than one defined by monotony.

The Brain's Internal Rhythm

Key Insight 3

Narrator: Is it possible that some people are just naturally "faster" or "slower" than others? Wittmann explores the fascinating idea that each of us might have a personal brain rhythm that dictates our speed of perception. This concept is brought to life through the historical story of John Franklin, a 19th-century English explorer who was famously slow. As a child, he couldn't catch a ball because he perceived its motion too slowly. His teacher, Dr. Orme, even invented a device with a spinning disk to measure his perception, confirming that Franklin's "temporal resolution" was much slower than his peers. Yet, this slowness was also a strength; his deliberate and thorough thinking made him a successful navigator and leader.

Modern science has found evidence for this idea. Researchers can measure a person's "temporal order threshold"—the minimum time gap required to tell which of two stimuli, like a flash of light or a sound, came first. For most young people, this is between 20 and 60 milliseconds. This threshold suggests the brain processes the world in discrete chunks of time. The German scientist Ernst Pöppel proposed that the brain operates on a fundamental rhythm of about 30 milliseconds, bundling all sensory input within that window into a single, simultaneous moment of "now." This internal rhythm may be the very foundation upon which our entire experience of time is built.

The Three-Second Present

Key Insight 4

Narrator: While the brain may process sensory data in tiny millisecond packets, our conscious experience of the "present moment" is significantly longer. Wittmann argues that we live in a "three-second present." This is the window of time in which our brain integrates sights, sounds, and thoughts into a coherent, flowing experience. Think of the Beatles' song "Hey Jude." As soon as you hear "Hey," your mind anticipates "Jude." When you hear "Jude," the memory of "Hey" is still active. The two words are bound together in a single perceptual unit that exists within this three-second window. This is how we understand sentences, appreciate musical phrases, and perceive continuous motion rather than a series of disjointed snapshots.

This concept is not just theoretical; it appears everywhere. The reversal of an optical illusion like the Necker cube happens, on average, every three seconds. Lines in poetry and musical phrases across cultures often fall into this three-second structure. This window is the domain of our working memory, which links these individual moments into a greater whole, creating our sense of a continuous self moving through time. By practicing mindfulness—paying deliberate, non-judgmental attention to the present—we can become more aware of this three-second unit of experience, effectively slowing down our perceived pace of life and feeling more present and in control.

The Body as the Ultimate Timekeeper

Key Insight 5

Narrator: For decades, scientists searched for a single "clock" in the brain responsible for our sense of time. Wittmann presents a radical and compelling alternative: there is no single clock, because the entire body functions as our timekeeper. Our sense of time, he argues, arises directly from our perception of our own bodily states. This is called interoception—the awareness of internal signals like our heartbeat, breathing, and gut feelings.

Experiments in sensory deprivation tanks vividly illustrate this. When subjects float in body-temperature water in total darkness and silence, external cues are eliminated. Yet, the sense of time doesn't vanish. It persists because they can still feel their own body—their breathing, their heart beating. In fact, fMRI studies have shown a direct link between time perception and a brain region called the insular cortex, which is the hub for interoception. In one study, the longer a subject listened to a tone, the more activity increased in their insular cortex, as if the brain were "counting" heartbeats to measure duration. Further research confirmed this: individuals who are better at accurately counting their own heartbeats without checking their pulse are also more accurate at estimating time intervals. Our feeling of time is not an abstract mental construct; it is the feeling of life itself, the rhythm of our own physiology. We don't just perceive time; we are time.

Conclusion

Narrator: The single most important takeaway from Felt Time is that our experience of time is not a passive observation of a clock on the wall, but an active, biological process generated from within. Time is not something that happens to us; it is an emergent property of our own consciousness, grounded in the rhythms of our body and the richness of our memories. Our sense of self and our sense of time are inseparable.

This realization is both profound and empowering. It challenges us to stop seeing time as an enemy to be managed or a resource to be saved, and instead view it as a medium to be shaped. If routine and a lack of novelty truly make our lives feel shorter, then the path to a subjectively longer, fuller life is clear. The challenge, then, is not to find more hours in the day, but to fill the hours we have with more life. What new experience, big or small, will you seek out this week to consciously slow down your felt time and make your memories richer?

00:00/00:00