Aibrary Logo
Podcast thumbnail

What Makes Us Human?

11 min

Introduction

Narrator: What happens when a chimpanzee sees a stick? It might see a tool for reaching a termite mound or a weapon for defense. But what happens when a human sees a stick? They might see the same things, but they might also see a lever, a spear, a component for a shelter, a line to be drawn in the sand, or even a symbol of leadership. This chasm between perceiving the world as it is and imagining what it could be lies at the heart of one of the most profound questions we can ask: What makes us human? In the thought-provoking collection of essays, What Makes Us Human?, editor Charles Pasternak assembles a team of leading experts from science, philosophy, and anthropology to tackle this very puzzle. The book reveals that there is no single, simple answer, but rather a complex mosaic of interconnected abilities that together define our species.

The Mind's Eye - Language, Time Travel, and Imagined Worlds

Key Insight 1

Narrator: While many species communicate, humans possess a unique cognitive toolkit that transforms communication into a gateway to other worlds. Scholars like Michael Corballis and Thomas Suddendorf argue that a key human adaptation is "mental time travel"—the ability to vividly re-experience the past and simulate the future. This isn't just about remembering facts; it's about re-living moments and planning detailed scenarios. This capacity, they suggest, is deeply intertwined with language, which may have evolved specifically to share these non-present events with others.

This ability to detach from the immediate present allows for what Robin Dunbar describes as our most profound distinction: the capacity to live in an imagined world. This is the foundation of literature, religion, and science. To understand its complexity, Dunbar points to Shakespeare's Othello. For an audience to follow the plot, they must engage in what is called higher-order intentionality. The audience must understand that Shakespeare intends for them to believe that Iago wants Othello to think that Desdemona is in love with Cassio. This nested understanding of multiple minds and their beliefs is a cognitive feat far beyond other apes. It is this ability to construct and navigate these intricate social and fictional realities that allows humans to build the vast, shared intellectual worlds we call culture.

The Social Superorganism - Imitation, Memes, and the Deep Social Mind

Key Insight 2

Narrator: Our species' journey was not a solo endeavor. A crucial turning point, as argued by Susan Blackmore, was the development of true imitation. While other animals learn, humans copy actions and sounds with such fidelity that it allows for a second kind of evolution: cultural evolution. Blackmore introduces the concept of "memes"—units of culture like ideas, tunes, or fashions—that replicate themselves by jumping from mind to mind. From this perspective, our large brains and capacity for language didn't just evolve for our own benefit; they co-evolved under pressure from the memes themselves, which favored hosts who were better at copying and spreading them.

This idea is powerfully illustrated by a "just-so" story of music's origin. Imagine an early hominid idly tapping a stick on a log. The rhythm is catchy. Others nearby hear it and, through an innate drive to imitate, begin tapping along. Soon, different rhythms and hummed tunes compete for attention. The most infectious ones spread, becoming the basis for a musical culture. This "memetic drive" then creates a new selective pressure, favoring individuals with brains better suited for perceiving and creating music. Culture, in this view, is not just something we create; it's a force that creates us. This aligns with Andrew Whiten's concept of a "deep social mind," an adaptive complex where culture, mind-reading, cooperation, and language work together, creating a formidable "socio-cognitive niche" that allowed our physically vulnerable ancestors to out-compete specialized predators and thrive.

The Technological Spark - Causal Belief and the Mastery of the Physical World

Key Insight 3

Narrator: Humans don't just live in a social world; we actively reshape our physical one. According to Lewis Wolpert, the engine of this transformation is a belief in physical cause and effect. This is not merely learning by association; it's a true understanding of underlying principles. This cognitive leap is what enables technology.

The difference is starkly illustrated in experiments with chimpanzees. In one study, chimps were presented with a clear tube containing a treat, with a trap in the middle. To get the treat, they had to push it from the end that would avoid the trap. Over dozens of trials, the chimps never grasped the causal principle, performing no better than chance. They couldn't form a mental model of the unseen force of gravity and how the trap worked. A human child, by contrast, quickly understands the concept. This fundamental ability to reason about cause and effect allowed our ancestors to move beyond using simple found objects as tools. It enabled them to fashion complex tools, like the Acheulian hand-axe, which required planning, foresight, and an understanding of how striking one stone against another would produce a desired shape. This causal belief was the prerequisite for all future technology, from controlling fire to building spacecraft.

The Environmental Forge - How Climate Change Drove Human Evolution

Key Insight 4

Narrator: Our unique cognitive abilities did not develop in a vacuum. Geologist Stephen Oppenheimer argues compellingly that the primary driver of our evolution was a fierce and unpredictable environment, specifically the dramatic climate shifts in Africa. Around 2.5 million years ago, a cooling and drying trend caused the forests to recede and be replaced by vast grasslands. This environmental pressure was a crucible for our ancestors.

This new, open landscape favored bipedalism, which freed the hands for carrying food and, eventually, tools. It also created a new set of challenges that selected for larger, more adaptable brains. The most significant period of brain growth in the Homo genus occurred during a time of intense climatic adversity. This challenges the idea that brain growth was driven solely by social competition or tool use. Instead, Oppenheimer suggests that a broader adaptation to environmental stress was the catalyst. Our ancestors were molded by the weather, forced to become more innovative, more cooperative, and more intelligent simply to survive the harsh realities of the expanding African savannah. Human expansions out of Africa and subsequent technological innovations often coincided with periods of climate amelioration, showing that our relationship with the environment has been a constant engine of change throughout our history.

The Symbolic Revolution - The Emergence of a New Kind of Mind

Key Insight 5

Narrator: The story of human evolution is not one of slow, steady, linear progress. As paleoanthropologist Ian Tattersall explains, the archaeological record shows long periods of stasis punctuated by sudden bursts of innovation. This suggests that our most defining trait—symbolic thought—was not gradually fine-tuned by natural selection. Instead, it likely emerged.

Emergence is the idea that a chance addition to a system can produce something entirely new and unexpected. Tattersall proposes that the biological potential for symbolic thought—the hardware—may have appeared with the arrival of anatomically modern Homo sapiens around 200,000 years ago, perhaps as a byproduct of other developmental changes in the brain. However, this potential lay dormant until it was "unlocked" by a cultural innovation, most likely the invention of language.

The evidence for this is explosive. For tens of thousands of years, early Homo sapiens behaved much like their Neanderthal cousins. Then, around 40,000 years ago in Europe, there was a creative explosion. These Cro-Magnon people began creating spectacular cave paintings, carving intricate figurines, and crafting musical instruments like vulture-bone flutes. This wasn't just an improvement on old behaviors; it was a new way of being, a world mentally reconstructed through symbols. This symbolic capacity, Tattersall argues, was not predicted by anything that came before it. It made us an entity of a totally new kind, capable of the reflection, art, and complex societies that define the human condition.

Conclusion

Narrator: Ultimately, What Makes Us Human? demonstrates that our uniqueness is not a single attribute but an intricate and emergent property. We are not just the ape that talks, or the ape that thinks about the future, or the ape that makes complex tools. We are the species where all these capacities—and more—collided and co-evolved, creating a "deep social mind" capable of symbolic thought and unprecedented cultural transmission. The book's most powerful takeaway is that humanity is a complex adaptive system, a product of a feedback loop between our genes, our environment, our minds, and the very cultures we invent.

This leaves us with a profound and challenging thought. If our minds are so deeply shaped by the culture we inherit and create—by the "memes" we choose to spread—what is our responsibility for the future of the human mind? The book suggests we are not just passive products of evolution, but active participants in its ongoing story, armed with a symbolic consciousness that gives us an almost infinite range of potentialities, for both good and ill.

00:00/00:00