
The Myth of You
14 minGolden Hook & Introduction
SECTION
Michael: You know that feeling of a core 'you' that's been there your whole life? The one that connects the kid in the playground to the person listening right now? Kevin: Oh yeah, absolutely. It feels like the one constant thing. My personality has changed, my body has changed, but there's this central 'me' that's just… there. The witness to it all. Michael: Well, today we’re exploring a book that argues that 'you' is the most convincing trick your brain ever played on itself. Kevin: Whoa, hold on. Are you saying I'm not real? That this whole conversation is just a figment of my… non-existent imagination? That’s a bit much for a Tuesday morning, Michael. Michael: (Laughs) Not exactly that you're not real, but that what you think you are is the illusion. And that trick is the central idea in Julian Baggini's fantastic book, The Ego Trick. Kevin: Baggini is a great guide for this. I read that his PhD was actually on personal identity. So this isn't just a passing interest for him; it's his life's work, distilled into something we can all grapple with. Michael: Exactly. And he blends philosophy with these incredible, real-life stories from neuroscience and psychology to make his case. It's why the book is so highly regarded—it’s not just abstract theory. It gets right into the messy, fascinating business of what it means to be a person. Kevin: Okay, I'm intrigued and slightly terrified. Where do we even start with dismantling my entire existence?
The Self as a Construction: Deconstructing the 'Pearl'
SECTION
Michael: Let's start with a classic. Have you heard of the Ship of Theseus? Kevin: Vaguely. It's a philosophical puzzle, right? The one where a ship has all its wooden planks replaced one by one. The question is, is it still the same ship at the end? Michael: Precisely. And if the old planks are collected and reassembled, which one is the real Ship of Theseus? There's no easy answer, because our concept of 'sameness' is fuzzy. Baggini argues we are just like that ship. We think there's a core, essential part of us—what he calls the 'pearl view' of the self. This idea that deep down, there's an unchanging pearl of 'me-ness'. Kevin: That’s exactly the feeling I was talking about. The witness. The pearl. It feels so real. Michael: It does. But neuroscience and some truly bizarre historical accidents suggest that pearl might not exist. Let me tell you about a man named Phineas Gage. Kevin: I think I've heard this name. This is not a happy story, is it? Michael: Not at all. It's 1848, Vermont. Phineas Gage is a 25-year-old railroad foreman. He's known as a great guy—efficient, capable, well-balanced, a favorite of his men and his bosses. One afternoon, he's using a tamping iron—a three-and-a-half-foot long, thirteen-pound metal rod—to pack explosive powder into a rock. Kevin: I can already see where this is going. Michael: A spark ignites the powder. The explosion drives the tamping iron straight up through his head. It enters under his left cheekbone, passes behind his eye, tears through the front part of his brain—the frontal lobe—and exits out the top of his skull, landing some eighty feet away. Kevin: My god. He died instantly, surely. Michael: That's the astonishing part. He didn't. He was thrown back, convulsed a bit, but was speaking and walking within minutes. He survived. But the man who survived was not the man who had the accident. His friends and family said he was "no longer Gage." The polite, reliable man was gone. In his place was someone who was profane, impatient, disrespectful, and couldn't stick to any plan. The 'pearl' of Phineas Gage, his very character, seemed to have been blasted out of his head along with that chunk of his brain. Kevin: Wow. So if a piece of your brain can be destroyed and take 'you' with it, then the self isn't some non-physical soul or a metaphysical pearl. It's physically constructed. It's a product of the brain's wiring. Michael: That’s the inescapable conclusion. And it gets even stranger. Neuroscientists have studied patients whose brain hemispheres have been surgically severed to treat severe epilepsy. These are the famous 'split-brain' patients. In experiments, you can show an image to just one hemisphere. If you show the right hemisphere—which controls the left hand but has no language—a picture of a key, the person can't tell you what they saw. They'll say "I saw nothing." Kevin: Because the language part of the brain, the left hemisphere, genuinely saw nothing. Michael: Exactly. But if you ask them to pick up the object they saw with their left hand, they will correctly pick up the key. The left hemisphere, the one that's talking, is completely bewildered. It will literally invent a reason for why the hand did that, like, "Oh, I must have just felt like picking up a key." The brain's press secretary, as they call it, creates a story to maintain the illusion of a single, unified command center. But the experiments show there isn't one. There are at least two semi-independent consciousnesses in one skull. Kevin: That is deeply unsettling. So there's no CEO in my head. It's more like a committee, and they're not always in the same meeting. Michael: A committee that's very, very good at pretending it's a single person. That's the beginning of the ego trick.
The Fragile Pillars of Identity: Memory and Character
SECTION
Kevin: Okay, so if it's not a physical 'pearl' in the brain, maybe our identity is our memories, right? The classic idea from the philosopher John Locke. I am who I am because of what I remember. My identity is the story my memories tell. Michael: That's the next logical place to look for the self. And it's a huge part of it. But Baggini argues that this pillar is also much shakier than we think. First, our memories are not reliable recordings of the past. They are reconstructions. Kevin: What do you mean, reconstructions? Michael: I mean they change every time you access them. The psychologist Elizabeth Loftus did a famous experiment where she showed people a fake ad for Disneyland. The ad featured Bugs Bunny. Kevin: Wait, but Bugs Bunny is a Warner Brothers character. He would never be at Disneyland. Michael: You'd think that would be a dead giveaway. Yet, after seeing the fake ad, up to a third of the subjects claimed they remembered meeting Bugs Bunny at Disneyland as a child. Their brains had seamlessly written this fiction into their personal history. Our memory is more of a creative storyteller than a precise archivist. Kevin: That’s… alarming. So my cherished childhood memories might be part-fiction, edited by my brain's internal marketing department? Michael: It's very likely. But the bigger challenge to memory as the seat of identity comes from cases of profound memory loss. Baggini tells the deeply moving story of a man he calls Robert, whose wife, Linda, has late-stage dementia. They've been married for 42 years. Kevin: Oh, this is going to be tough. Michael: It is. Linda is only in her sixties, but the disease has been swift. She no longer recognizes Robert. She can't speak. He holds her hand, and he says there's sometimes a flicker, a tiny response that suggests he isn't just a stranger. But the person he married, the person who shares 42 years of memories with him, is gone. He says, "She is with me and not with me. We are together and we are apart." Kevin: That's heartbreaking. So is Linda still Linda? If her memories are gone, is the 'person' gone too? Michael: That's the devastating question. Some caregivers feel the person they knew is no longer there. Others, like a care home manager Baggini interviews, believe dementia strips away all the baggage and what's left is the 'core person,' their essential personality. Kevin: Right, their character! Even if memories fade, their personality—whether they were kind, or funny, or grumpy—that remains. So maybe that's the pearl. Our character. Michael: Another good candidate! But psychology has some bad news for that idea, too. A series of famous experiments show that our character is shockingly situational. In the 'Good Samaritan' experiment, theology students at Princeton were asked to walk across campus to give a talk. Along the way, they passed an actor slumped in a doorway, groaning and in clear distress. Kevin: And these are theology students, training to be religious leaders. They're probably primed for compassion. I bet most of them stopped to help. Michael: You'd think so. But the researchers added a twist. They told one group they were late, another they were on time, and a third they had plenty of time. Of those who were early, 63% helped. Of those on time, 45%. And of those who were told they were in a hurry? Only 10% stopped to help. Some literally stepped over the suffering man on their way to give a talk about the Good Samaritan. Kevin: Come on! That's awful. So their 'character'—their compassion, their morality—was less important than whether they thought they were running a few minutes late? Michael: It seems so. The situation, not their inherent character, was the biggest predictor of their behavior. From the Milgram obedience experiments, where ordinary people administered what they thought were fatal electric shocks just because a man in a lab coat told them to, to the Stanford Prison Experiment, where college students became sadistic guards, the evidence is overwhelming. Our character is not a fixed, internal trait. It's a response to our environment. Kevin: So there's no solid pearl, our memories are editable fictions, and our character can be flipped by a ticking clock. This is not looking good for Team Self. What is left?
Living Without a Soul: The 'Ego Trick' and Its Implications
SECTION
Michael: And that brings us to the heart of it all. If there's no pearl, no perfect memory, no fixed character... what's left? This is the 'Ego Trick' itself. The trick is not that we don't exist. We do. The trick is that we misinterpret the feeling of being a unified self for a thing that is a unified self. Kevin: Okay, break that down for me. That sounds like a philosophical tongue-twister. Michael: Think of a waterfall. Is a waterfall a 'thing'? You can point to it, it has a name, it persists over time. But it's not a static object. It's a process. It's a pattern created by countless water molecules constantly flowing through it. The waterfall is real, but it's a verb, not a noun. Baggini argues the self is like that. It's a real, persistent pattern, a process of consciousness, but it's made of constantly changing thoughts, feelings, memories, and physical states. There is no single water molecule that is the waterfall, and there is no single thought or feeling that is you. Kevin: I like that analogy. The waterfall. So I'm not a solid statue; I'm a persistent pattern. A process. An ongoing event. That feels… lighter, somehow. Less rigid. Michael: Exactly. It's what some philosophers call a 'bundle theory' of the self. We're just a bundle of perceptions and experiences, tied together by psychological continuity. And this view has profound implications for how we live. To make this idea really land, Baggini brings in the work of Douglas Hofstadter, who wrote the incredible book Gödel, Escher, Bach. Kevin: I know that one. A beast of a book. What does he say? Michael: Hofstadter has a very personal and moving take on this. He lost his wife, Carol, very suddenly to a brain tumor. He was devastated. But as a bundle theorist, he didn't see her as a single 'thing' that was suddenly extinguished. He describes a person as a 'strange loop'—a pattern of thought and belief that is constantly feeding back on itself. And when you love someone deeply, you spend years learning their strange loop. You internalize their way of thinking, their humor, their values. Kevin: You can finish their sentences. You know exactly how they'd react to a piece of news. Michael: Precisely. Hofstadter says that a huge part of Carol's pattern, her 'soul' as he calls it, had been copied into his own brain. So when she died, she wasn't entirely gone. A part of her was still running on his neural hardware. He writes that after her death, he felt there wasn't an unbridgeable gap between them, because "each of them was a spread-out, distributed, partially overlapping, interpenetrating, dynamic pattern." Kevin: Wow. That's... a beautiful and heartbreaking way to think about it. It reframes loss. It's not that a 'thing' is gone forever, but a pattern, a process, has ended, yet echoes of it remain in the people who loved them. It's a kind of impermanent afterlife. Michael: It is. It's a way of understanding connection and loss that doesn't require a soul or a pearl. It just requires accepting that we are these complex, interwoven patterns. Our boundaries are more porous than we think.
Synthesis & Takeaways
SECTION
Kevin: So, after all this deconstruction, where does Baggini leave us? If I'm a waterfall, a bundle, a story… what do I do with that knowledge? Does it change how I order my coffee in the morning? Michael: (Laughs) It might! The book argues that this isn't just an abstract intellectual exercise. Understanding the ego trick can fundamentally change your relationship with your own life. If you don't have a fixed, essential self, then you don't have to be a slave to the person you were yesterday. You have more freedom to shape the person you will be tomorrow. Your identity isn't something you find, it's something you create through your actions and choices. Kevin: That’s a really empowering thought. It shifts the focus from 'Who am I?' to 'Who do I want to become?' Michael: Exactly. And it can change our view of death. Derek Parfit, another philosopher in this tradition, said that realizing he was a bundle of experiences made him fear death less. It's not that a precious, singular thing will be annihilated. It's that a particular series of experiences will come to an end, while other parts of the pattern—our influence, our work, the memories in others—continue for a while. It's less like a full stop and more like an ellipsis. Kevin: It seems the ego trick isn't a cruel joke, then. It's a useful feature. It gives us a sense of coherence so we can navigate the world, but understanding the mechanics behind it gives us a kind of freedom. Michael: That's the perfect summary. So the ego trick isn't a lie to be exposed, but a feature of our consciousness to be understood. We are not solid things, but ongoing stories, woven from memory, biology, and our connections to others. Kevin: It leaves you with a powerful question: If you are a story, what kind of story do you want to write for yourself? Michael: We'd love to hear your thoughts on this. Does this idea of a 'bundle self' resonate with you, or does it feel unsettling? Let us know. Michael: This is Aibrary, signing off.