Aibrary Logo
Podcast thumbnail

Minds Built Twice Over

12 min

The Octopus, the Sea, and the Deep Origins of Consciousness

Golden Hook & Introduction

SECTION

Michael: Most people searching for alien intelligence look to the stars. That’s a mistake. The most alien mind we’ll ever meet is already here on Earth, hiding in plain sight, and its last common ancestor with us was a brainless, flat worm. Kevin: Whoa, hold on. A brainless flat worm? That sounds less like an ancestor and more like an insult. What on earth are you talking about? Are we related to something that just wiggled on the seafloor? Michael: We are, and so is the creature with that alien mind: the octopus. That's the mind-bending premise of Other Minds: The Octopus, the Sea, and the Deep Origins of Consciousness by Peter Godfrey-Smith. Kevin: Ah, okay, the picture is getting clearer. And Godfrey-Smith is the perfect person to write this, right? He's not just a highly-regarded philosopher of science; he's an avid scuba diver. He's actually met these creatures face-to-face, which gives the book this incredible, personal dimension that's won it so much praise. Michael: Exactly. It's not just theory; it's observation. It’s philosophy done with a wetsuit on. And that's where our story begins, with this idea of a second genesis of the mind, a completely separate path to intelligence.

The Great Split: An Alien Intelligence on Earth

SECTION

Kevin: Okay, so "a second genesis." That sounds huge. Unpack that for me. How separate are we talking? Michael: We're talking about a fundamental split in the tree of life. You have to go back about 600 million years. At that point, the ancestor of all complex animals, including us and the octopus, was this simple, worm-like creature. It had no complex brain, maybe just some light-sensitive spots. It was basically a little digestive tube that could move. Kevin: So, a very, very distant cousin. Michael: Extremely distant. After that ancestor, the evolutionary path forked. One branch led to vertebrates—fish, amphibians, reptiles, birds, and eventually, us mammals. The other branch led to invertebrates, and down one of those paths, you get to the mollusks, and then to the cephalopods: squid, cuttlefish, and our main character, the octopus. Kevin: So our lineages haven't crossed paths for 600 million years? That’s an almost unimaginable amount of time. Michael: Precisely. And that's why Godfrey-Smith makes this profound point. He says, and I'm quoting loosely here, "If we can make contact with cephalopods as sentient beings, it is not because of a shared history, not because of kinship, but because evolution built minds twice over." Kevin: Wow. Evolution built minds twice over. That gives me chills. But what does a 'mind' even mean in this context? We're not talking about an octopus writing poetry or doing calculus, are we? Michael: Not at all. And that's a crucial distinction Godfrey-Smith makes. He's less interested in complex, human-style consciousness and more interested in the origins of subjective experience. The simple, raw feeling of what it's like to be something. What does it feel like to be hungry? To feel the cold water? To see a predator? Kevin: Ah, so the basic, raw sensation of existence. Michael: Exactly. He argues this likely began during the Cambrian explosion, about 540 million years ago. This was a period of intense evolutionary innovation. Animals developed eyes, claws, and shells. And for the first time, they were actively hunting each other. He has this great line: "From this point on, the mind evolved in response to other minds." You had to be smart enough to eat, and smart enough not to be eaten. Kevin: That makes sense. It became an arms race of perception and action. But I have to ask, and I know some critics have raised this, isn't this all a bit speculative? How can we possibly know what a worm or an early octopus felt? Michael: It's a fair question, and the book is very honest about the limits of our knowledge. Godfrey-Smith isn't claiming to have definitive answers. Instead, he's using the octopus as a philosophical tool. He's building a case based on evolutionary logic and observable behavior. He’s not saying an octopus is conscious in the way we are, but that its biology and behavior strongly suggest a rich inner world. It forces us to confront the possibility that our kind of mind isn't the only kind. Kevin: Okay, so it’s a thought experiment grounded in biology. I can get behind that. It’s less about providing answers and more about asking better questions. Michael: That’s the perfect way to put it. And when you see what these 'other minds' can actually do, the questions get even wilder.

Mischief, Craft, and the Embodied Mind

SECTION

Kevin: I'm glad you said that, because I want to hear about the mischief. The book is full of these amazing, almost unbelievable stories of octopus intelligence. Lay some on me. Michael: Oh, the stories are fantastic. They show an intelligence that's not just smart, but crafty and curious. There are famous accounts from aquariums in New Zealand and Germany where octopuses learned to short-circuit the bright overhead lights they disliked. They would wait until no one was looking, then shoot jets of water at the lightbulbs, causing a short. In one case, it became so expensive to keep replacing the system that they had to release the octopus back into the wild. Kevin: It won! The octopus literally won its freedom by being a persistent, electrical-sabotaging pest. That's incredible. Michael: It gets even more personal. There's a story from a researcher, Jean Boal, who was feeding her octopuses thawed squid, which is apparently second-rate food for them. They prefer crabs. One octopus took the piece of squid, held it in its arm, and made a point of watching her. It then slowly, deliberately, moved across the tank to the outflow drain and, while maintaining eye contact, dumped the squid down the drain. Kevin: Come on! That's not just intelligence; that's communication. That's a food critic giving a one-star review in the most passive-aggressive way possible. It knew she was watching and performed the act for her. Michael: It's hard to interpret it any other way. And this is where the biology gets so fascinating. What allows for this kind of complex, crafty behavior is a nervous system that is completely alien to ours. A common octopus has about 500 million neurons. For comparison, a rat has about 200 million. So it's in the mammalian ballpark. But here's the kicker: two-thirds of those neurons are not in its central brain. They're in its arms. Kevin: Wait, the brain is in the arms? How does that work? Michael: It’s a decentralized system. Each arm can act semi-independently. It can touch, taste, and move on its own, gathering information and making decisions locally. The central brain acts more like a conductor, giving general commands like "move over there" or "explore that crevice," but the arms figure out the details. Kevin: So it's like a company with a CEO in the head office, but each department is a semi-autonomous startup that can run its own projects? Michael: That's a perfect analogy. And it leads to this mind-bending idea Godfrey-Smith explores, quoting another scientist, that "the octopus, in a sense, is disembodied." Its sense of self isn't neatly contained in its head. The body is the mind. The arms are thinking. This is what's called 'embodied cognition,' but taken to an absolute extreme. Kevin: That's so hard to wrap my head around. We experience ourselves as a single point of consciousness behind our eyes. The idea of having eight smart, thinking limbs that are part of you but also kind of... not you... is just bizarre. Michael: It is. And it explains their incredible physical abilities. They can change color, texture, and shape in an instant. They can pour their bodies through any hole larger than their beak. This "protean" or shape-shifting nature is possible because they don't have a rigid skeleton controlled by a central computer. They have a flexible, intelligent body. Kevin: Okay, so we have this incredibly intelligent, crafty, semi-decentralized being. A true evolutionary marvel. Which brings us to the most heartbreaking part of the book.

The Puzzle of a Brilliant, Short Life

SECTION

Kevin: This is all incredible. But the book points out this giant, heartbreaking paradox. Why would evolution go to all the trouble of building this super-complex, crafty, intelligent being... just for it to live for a year or two? What is the evolutionary point? Michael: It's a devastating question, and it's the puzzle that hangs over the entire book. Godfrey-Smith describes watching these magnificent giant cuttlefish, which he gets to know as individuals, enter a sudden, rapid decline at the end of their second winter. Their bodies literally start to fall apart. Their skin loses its magic, their eyes go cloudy, they lose limbs. It's a programmed death. Kevin: That’s so brutal. It feels like a waste. All that learning, all that experience, just gone in a flash. Michael: It does. And the evolutionary theory of aging that Godfrey-Smith explains is just as brutal. The key insight comes from a trade-off made hundreds of millions of years ago. The ancestors of the octopus had shells, like a nautilus does today. They were slow but well-protected. At some point, they ditched the shell. Kevin: Why would they do that? Michael: For speed, flexibility, and the ability to become active hunters. Losing the shell allowed them to develop jet propulsion and that incredible camouflage. It unlocked their intelligence. But it came at a huge cost: they became soft, squishy, delicious meals for basically everything in the ocean. They became incredibly vulnerable. Kevin: So they traded armor for brains. Michael: A perfect summary. And because their daily risk of being eaten was now so high, evolution favored a "live fast, die young" strategy. There's no evolutionary benefit in investing energy in long-term body maintenance—like cancer prevention or cellular repair—if you're almost certainly going to be eaten by a seal within a year anyway. It's better to pour all your energy into growing fast, getting smart enough to survive for a season, reproducing once, and then... you're done. Your body's warranty is up. Kevin: Wow, so their short lifespan is a direct consequence of their intelligence and vulnerability. They're intertwined. Michael: They are. But the book offers a fascinating exception that proves the rule. Researchers studying the deep sea off California discovered a species of octopus, Graneledone boreopacifica, in an environment with far fewer predators. They watched one female guard her eggs. She sat there, without eating, for 53 months. Kevin: Fifty-three months? That's over four and a half years! Michael: Longer than any other octopus is even thought to live. It shows that the octopus body isn't inherently programmed to fall apart after a year. The lifespan is "tuned" by the environment. In the high-risk, high-reward world of the shallow reefs, life is a sprint. In the cold, slow, safer world of the deep sea, it can be a marathon. Kevin: That deep-sea octopus is like a little glimmer of hope in a pretty grim story about aging. It shows what's possible when the pressure is off. Michael: It does. It shows that evolution is not a single story, but a constant negotiation with the circumstances of life and death.

Synthesis & Takeaways

SECTION

Michael: And that really brings us to the heart of what Other Minds is about. It’s a journey that starts with the great evolutionary split that gave us a parallel experiment in what a mind can be. It then shows us that this other mind is radically embodied, decentralized, and expressed through mischief and craft. And finally, it reveals that this brilliant existence, however brief, is a profound story about the evolutionary trade-offs between intelligence, vulnerability, and the preciousness of time. Kevin: That's a fantastic summary. So after all this, what does the octopus teach us about our own minds? It feels like the answer has to be more than just 'wow, octopuses are cool.' Michael: I think it's a lesson in intellectual humility. It teaches us that our way of being conscious—centralized in a brain, organized by language, obsessed with a sense of a single, enduring self—is just one possible way to be a mind. It's the model that worked for a highly social, land-based primate. The ocean, working with a completely different set of materials and pressures, created another. Kevin: So it de-centers us. It reminds us we're not the pinnacle of consciousness, just one version of it. Michael: Exactly. It reminds us that the world is filled with other minds, and maybe the first step to understanding them is to stop assuming they think, feel, or experience the world anything like we do. They are the true aliens among us, and they have so much to teach us about the sheer creative possibility of life. Kevin: That's a powerful thought to end on. What do you all think? Does the octopus feel like an alien, or a distant cousin? Let us know your thoughts on our social channels. We'd love to hear your perspective. Michael: This is Aibrary, signing off.

00:00/00:00